Win or lose, are you lucky or shrewd?

Recent polls shows that people put good returns down to skill while bad ones are blamed on bad luck. But now there are calls for a more scientific approach


Does luck trump skill in investment. Research indicates that returns are usually dictated more by chance than skill, and it’s hard to tell the difference between a good investor and a lucky one.

Bill Miller was a legendary figure in the US investment world, having beaten the S&P 500 15 years in a row between 1991 and 2005.

Christened the "greatest money manager of our time" by Fortune magazine in 2006, it all went wrong in 2008, when his shareholdings lost 55 of their value. By the time Miller announced his retirement in late 2011, his fund's assets had fallen from $20.1bn to $2.8bn, having underperformed markets for five of the six previous years.

Was Miller’s winning streak a product of dumb luck? Was he unlucky at the end of his career? Might it have been a bit of both? How do we know which money managers have been skilful, and which ones have been lucky?

READ MORE

"If you ask a bunch of people to flip coins, maybe one out of a thousand will flip heads 20 times in a row," says indexing guru John Bogle. "In our business, we'll declare him a genius."

Chance

Another advocate of index funds, Nobel economist

Eugene Fama

, agrees, saying investors don’t understand the effects of chance on returns.

In one study analysing 3,156 funds over the 1984-2006 period, Fama found that overall, actively managed funds underperformed markets. Only a tiny percentage – about 2 per cent – delivered results outside what could be expected by chance. Investors could profit by picking such funds, but there was one problem – the good funds were “indistinguishable from the lucky bad funds”. There was no way of telling for sure if the managers in question were highly skilled or lucky.

Outcome bias

Ordinary investors, unfortunately, often confuse brains with a bull market. In a recent study,

Self-Attribution Bias in Consumer Financial Decision-Making: How Investment Returns Affect Individuals’ Belief in Skill

, researchers asked clients of a brokerage firm if the recent performance of their portfolio accurately reflected their investment skills.

The higher recent returns were, the more they agreed with the statement. The poorer their returns, the less inclined they were to agree. In other words, good returns are the product of skill; bad returns are a case of bad luck.

If self-attribution bias is a problem, so is outcome bias. Many business books, for example, select a handful of top-performing companies and then look for secrets to their success.

Delusions

The need for a more scientific approach is outlined in

Halo Effect

, the acclaimed book by management expert

Philip Rosenzweig

, who criticises the “delusions” that characterise such thinking. One such example is the “delusion of connecting the winning dots”, whereby commentators look for the common features in successful companies, without comparing them against unsuccessful companies, who may well share the same traits.

There is the “delusion of absolute performance”; a company’s success, he notes, is down to what competitors do as well as the company itself. A firm might do everything right and still fail. Additionally, business gurus tend to mistake correlation for causation.

The same problems are explored in a provocative 2009 paper called Are 'Great' Companies Just Lucky? Evaluating 287 allegedly high-performing companies detailed in 13 major business books, the researchers found only about one in four was likely to be remarkable. The rest "were indistinguishable from mediocre firms catching lucky breaks". Success gurus "are simply imposing patterns on random data. That's not science – it's astrology".

Even if a company is great, of course, it doesn't mean it is a great stock. A study of Fortune's most admired companies, for example, found those with the worst ratings went on to earn better returns than the most admired firms.

Investors, warns money manager and behavioural finance expert Michael Mauboussin, are prone to a "creeping determinism", the belief that what happened was the only thing that could have happened. "When you see effects that are the consequence of luck, you misattribute them to skill," he says. It is a "very natural human reaction, but leads to faulty thinking in many instances".

Buffett and Bolton

Sceptics might point to investors like

Warren Buffett

. Surely his success was not down to luck? Few would make such a claim. Indeed, a study last year found Buffett’s returns are “neither luck nor magic”, but reward for his use of leverage “combined with a focus on cheap, safe, quality stocks”.

Few examples are so clear-cut, however, and problems can arise even in cases where there is strong evidence of skill. Take value investor Anthony Bolton, often dubbed the UK's answer to Buffett. Anyone who invested £1,000 (€1,231) with Bolton in 1979 would have seen it grow to around £147,000 (€181,002) in 2007.

Of course, no one knew anything about Bolton in 1979. Investment consultant and Smart Investing author Tim Hale gives the example of an investor who decided to bet on Bolton in 1989, by which time his outperformance was widely known. Three years later, that investor would have lagged the market by about 45 per cent. In 1996, Bolton was nearly level with the market, before falling back significantly in 1997 and 1998.

Not until 2000 did Bolton finally edge ahead of the market. “Eleven years of uncertainty – would you really have had the stomach to see this through?” asks Hale.

Losing on purpose?

The very fact one cannot be sure if returns are the product of luck or skill causes problems.

Michael Mauboussin suggests one test to help determine if there is any skill in an activity – just ask if you can lose on purpose.

“If you can lose on purpose, then there is some sort of skill. Investing is very interesting because it is difficult to build a portfolio that does a lot better than the benchmark.

“But it is also very hard, given the parameters, to build a portfolio that does a lot worse than the benchmark. What that tells you is that investing is far over to the luck side of the continuum.”

While dwelling on investment winners is problematic, given the unknown role of luck, a strong argument can be made for analysing the behaviour of consistent losers.

One study, Just Unlucky?, analysed returns from 8,621 individual portfolios from a German online broker.

Even before expenses were deducted, 89 per cent of investors exhibited “negative skill”.

They underperformed by 7.5 per cent annually, the study found, or 8.5 per cent after fees. Other studies have reached similar findings, ordinary investors typically dooming themselves due to overtrading of expensive glamour stocks.

Lucky break

Ironically, one of the best examples of the importance of luck comes from Warren Buffett’s mentor, legendary value investor

Benjamin Graham

.

In 1948, Graham, who normally preached the values of diversification, put around 20 per cent of his funds into insurer Geico.

The stock’s subsequent appreciation meant it began to look expensive by Graham’s criteria, but he never sold it. Graham broke all his own rules, and was rewarded for doing so – as the years passed, his Geico shareholding appreciated in value from some $700,000 (€513k) to over $1bn (€0.73bn).

That single purchase yielded more profits than the total of all other investments made over his lengthy career.

The moral? As Graham himself admitted: “One lucky break, or one supremely shrewd decision – can we tell them apart? – may count for more than a lifetime of journeyman efforts.”